Key points from Chapter Four of Diane
Ravitch’s Left Back: A Century of Battles Over School Reform are
as follows:
During World War I, psychometricians in the
United States administered intelligence tests to soldiers to determine whether
those serving were best fit for roles in the regular field of battle or as
officers. Both achievement tests and
intelligence tests had already been developed and used in a limited number of
schools. A leader in this effort was
Edward L. Thorndike of the Teachers College (Columbia University); the assessment that he had developed tested
for acuity in reading, arithmetic, spelling, handwriting, written composition,
geography, and other academic subjects.
In 1921, Ross Finner of the University of
Minnesota declared educational problems to be amenable to solutions via the
exact science of testing. Finner was
typical among psychologists of this period, and most school districts in the
United States were persuaded by the results and claims based on those results
made by psychometricians. School
administrators became convinced that standardized achievement and intelligence
tests were more reliable than tests devised by individual teachers; the standardized tests were also faster and
easier to grade than tests that had a subjective component, including long-form
essays.
These tests were put to use in the service of
curriculum differentiation and tracking according to counselor-determined
vocational destiny. These approaches
were consistent with the drive for social efficiency in an age of mass
production; students were evaluated for their capacity to perform functions in
the realms of business, factory, and the trades.
Thorndike was among the chief proponents of
testing for ability to determine intelligence, character, and skill; and he was convinced that such ability could
be thoroughly understood, controlled, and directed for the social good. Thorndike was certain that intelligence is
hereditary and immutable. He recognized
three main types of intelligence:
abstract, social, and mechanical;
his bias as to superiority was toward the abstract form of intelligence,
which he regarded as best able to determine high character. He was among those who had ready answers for
critics who thought his tenets antidemocratic;
he and others in fact thought that differentiation according to ability
and projected role in society best served the needs of a democracy. He asserted that “In the long run it has paid
the masses to be ruled by intelligence.”
The Early Days of IQ Testing
In the first years of the 20th century,
Francis Galton (a cousin of Charles Darwin) established the “Anthropometric
Laboratory” in London, testing for reaction time and mental acuity in human
subjects. Galton was a fervent believer
in the ability of the human race to improve mental capacity via astute
breeding; he held that various races and
nationalities have innate characteristics, and that some were more mentally
capable than others.
In France, Alfred Binet and his
colleague Theodore Simone sought to identify mentally defective children so as
to provide for the instruction most likely to serve their needs. They devised a test featuring tasks of
increasing difficulty pertinent to attention, memory, visual discrimination,
imagination, and verbal fluency. They
concluded that the most important component of intelligence is judgment,
involving practical sense, initiative, and adaptability. In his book, Modern Ideas About Children (published in 1909), Binet emphasized
the capacity of education to improve intelligence, rejecting the notion of
intelligence as fixed and immutable; but
in the aftermath of his death in 1910, those who drew upon his work did not hew
to the principle of the improvability of intelligence.
This was manifestly true of Henry
Goddard, who was much inspired by Binet’s work but put it to the service of
much more determinist applications.
Goddard was the resident director of the School for the Feebleminded in
Vineland, New Jersey. Coiner of the
term, “moron,” he was deeply concerned that the feebleminded were proliferating
in society and poised to do much harm.
In his 1912 book, The Kallikak Family, Goddard detailed the life trajectories of a
man who fathered children by two women, one who was feebleminded and another
who was mentally capable and from a family of high social standing. According to Goddard’s account, which struck
a resonant chord in the popular imagination, the children born of the
feebleminded manifested an array of mental and moral disorders; the children born of the woman of elevated
social standing proved to be respectable and of good character.
Eugenics, the purported science of
mental capability as distinguishable by race, had many proponents in the early
decades of the 20th century. Among
these were well-regarded professionals who sought to use eugenics for the
social good; proponents included birth
control advocate Margaret Sanger; psychologists
Robert Yerkes and Walter McDougal of Harvard;
Edward L. Thorndike of Teachers College (Columbia University); and Lewis Terman of Stanford University. But during the 1920s, eugenics attracted
racists and nativists, whose views came to be discredited by the use of
eugenics by the Nazis.
The influence of eugenics could be
seen in an emerging view that those who were determined to be mentally
defective should be permanently institutionalized and sterilized. Thirty states legalized the sterilization of
people determined to be insane, feebleminded, or criminal.
Lewis Terman developed the
Standford-Binet Intelligence Test for the measurement of a single Intelligence Quotient
(I. Q.) for each individual tested. The
I. Q. score was determined by a person’s mental age, divided by actual age, and
multiplied by one hundred. In examining
I. Q. scores, Terman asserted that scores in the 70s and 80s were typical for
Mexicans, negroes, and those of Spanish-Indian ancestry in the American
Southwest. He lamented that there seemed
to be little prospect of limiting propagation among these groups and considered
the offspring of these groups likely to cause harm to society.
Psychologists and the Army Tests
Robert Yerkes (President of the American
Psychological Association and Chair of the Psychology Committee of the National
Research Council) was the designer of the army tests that so inspired the
movement for intelligence testing. He
designed two tests, an Alpha Test for those who were literate and a Beta Test
for illiterates; the former included
arithmetic problems, analogies, synonyms, antonyms, and various information-based
questions; the latter featured tasks for
the completion of incomplete drawings and the comparison of shapes.
Lewis Terman’s status among psychometricians
was buttressed by his election in 1922 as president of the American
Psychological Association. Along with
Elwood P. Cubberly, Terman edited a major textbook series published by Houghton
Mifflin that envisioned a future of intelligence testing that was highly
compatible with the aims of educational psychologists and administrators regarding
curricular differentiation, vocational guidance, and social efficiency. Terman became a leading designer of
intelligence testing for the schools.
The Rockefeller Foundation bestowed a $25,000
grant to the National Research Council for adapting the army tests for
administration in schools. During
1919-1920, Terman, Yerkes, and Thorndike joined Guy M. Whipple (professor,
University of Michigan) in the design of tests for grades 3-8; by the mid-1920s, there were 75 different
mental ability tests used for assessing students of all ages. Evolutionary biologist Stephen Jay Gould has
observed that anyone who constructed such an assessment was careful not to
diverge from the approach espoused by Terman.
Terman’s key guiding principles were that 1) educability limits of any
individual were apparent by age five or six;
2) unless those limits were used to guide programming, neither the
gifted nor the dull could thrive; and 3)
one-third of students lacked the ability to perform adequately according to a
commonly taught academic curriculum. The
solution was to individualize instruction or to group students
homogenously; Terman contended that the
former was not practical, given the rise in enrollments, so that grouping students
according to ability was the answer. His
suggested groupings were five in number:
1) gifted; 2) bright;
3) average; 4) slow; and 5) special.
Terman recommended tracking from the sixth or
seventh grade, with careful attention to I. Q. scores. Terman’s scale to be used for academic tracking
based on I. Q scores forecast vocation or profession as follows: 1) below 70 I. Q. >>>>>
unskilled laborer; 2) 70-80 I. Q.
>>>>> semiskilled laborer;
3) 80-100 I. Q. >>>>> skilled laborer or ordinary
clerical worker; 4) 100-115 I. Q.
>>>>> semiprofessional;
5) above 115 I. Q. >>>>> professional or corporate
manager. In a speech to the National
Education Association is 1923, Terman blasted his critics as sentimentalists
who denied the reality of individual differences, maintaining that democracy is
best served by citizens who understand their roles and seek work appropriate to
their ability.
The Political Uses of the Army Tests
Assessing available data, leading
psychologists concluded that the average mental age among draftees in World War
I was thirteen (13) or fourteen (14). When
it became known that those of Nordic origins scored much higher than those of
southern and eastern European origins, the test results became fodder for
racists and nationalists who sought immigration restrictions. Eugenicists argued for immigration
restrictions for southern and eastern Europeans. Progressive sociologist Edward A. Ross,
well-known as a social control theorist, asserted that based on his
observations and reading of the data, eastern and southern Europeans were so
inferior as to dilute the American racial stock; furthermore, they were culturally backward
and inclined toward dangerous ideologies such as anarchy and socialism. One
commentator proclaimed that “We are being swamped by the off-scourge of
Europe. Those at the lower end of the
intellectual scale have brought their social customs, their language, their
political ideals. They cannon assimilate
our ideals… They cannot become citizens
in the highest meaning of the word. We
do not need the ignorant, the mentally feeble, the moron.”
In 1922, Lantrhop Stoddard in his book, The Revolt against Civilization: The Menace of the Under Man, asserted that almost one half of the United
States population would never get beyond the mental age of 13 or 14 years. That same year, research analyst Cornelia
James Cannon wrote an article for The
Atlantic Monthly in which she asserted that one half of the draftees in
World War I were morons and 22 percent were of even more inferior
intelligence. She read the data to show
that intellectual inferiority was disproportionately found among immigrants
from Poland, Italy, and Russia. She
wrote, “They are a menace that may encompass our mass destruction.”
A year later, Terman authored an article of
his own for The Atlantic Monthly. He asserted that the average mental age for
the native-born American was 13-14 years and that for the foreign-born was less
than twelve (12). He maintained that
Negroes, Italians, and Poles dominated the lower scores, while the higher
scores were dominated by native-born whites and immigrants from England and
Scotland. Terman stressed the importance
of race for national destiny, as did Carl C. Brigham (Princeton University
professor) in his 1923 A Study of
American Intelligence.
Other books that added to the momentum for
such assertions regarding racial stock and implications for the national
destiny included Charles W. Gould’s America: A Family Matter, Madison Grant’s The Passing
of the Great Race, and Willliam Z. Ripley’s Races of Europe. Historians
would come to regard Grant as the nation’s most influential racist, followed by
Lanthrop Stoddard. The latter in 1922
established the Goddard Society, along with Edward L. Thorndike and Henry F.
Osborne (of the American Museum of Natural History).
Carl C. Brigham’s classification of races
gained much attention and induced similar categorizations; the classifications were three in number: Nordic (northern Europeans, notably the
populations of Sweden, Belgium, the Netherlands, and England); Alpine (Romania, Russia, Poland, and
Austro-Hungary); and Mediterranean (Turkey,
Portugal, Spain, Greece, and Italy).
Brigham maintained that Germans were forty percent (40%) Nordic and
sixty percent (60%) Alpine; French,
thirty percent (30%) Nordic, fifty-five percent (55%) Alpine; and fifteen percent
(15%) Mediterranean; Irish, thirty
percent (30%) Nordic and seventy percent (70%) Mediterranean.
Maintaining that the purpose of school should
be to select and retain the most intellectually capable students, Robert Yerkes
analyzed the data as pertinent to school performance. He concluded that among students in the
relevant ages, only fifty percent (50%) were capable of performing well at a
first-rate high school; and that only
ten percent (10%) were capable of pursuing a B. A. at the college level. Yerkes found ingenious ways of reasoning away
indications that student performance actually rose as the ill-regarded
immigrant populations lived two decades in the United States; and that students who persisted in their
education through high school did improve results on assessments. Yerkes argued that those who were able to
persist were the most capable, so that schooling did little to improve
intellectual ability or normed achievement, which depended on native
intelligence.
Terman noted that many teachers objected to his assertion that education could not improve intellectual performance. He ridiculed them with the statement that
their reaction was nothing more than “deep-seated and blind faith that anything
was possible for a child.”
No comments:
Post a Comment